Transport-, Bygnings- og Boligudvalget 2017-18
TRU Alm.del Bilag 136
Offentligt
1848319_0001.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE
UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN DENMARK
USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
DECEMBER 2017
AALBORG UNIVERSITY
Public reactions to
drone use in residential
and public areas
–1–
UNIVERSITY OF
SOUTHERN DENMARK
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
COLOPHON
Public reactions to drone use in residential and public areas
AUTHORED BY:
Domen Bajde, University of Southern Denmark
Niklas Woermann, University of Southern Denmark
Maja Hojer Bruun, Aalborg University
Rasmus Gahrn-Andersen, University of Southern Denmark
Jannek K. Sommer, University of Southern Denmark
Mikkel Nøjgaard, University of Southern Denmark
Sofie H. Christensen, University of Southern Denmark
Heiko Kirschner, University of Southern Denmark
Rune Hagel Skaarup Jensen, Aalborg University
Jan Hendrik Bucher, University of Southern Denmark
–2–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
SUMMARY OF MAIN RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
RESEARCH DESIGN & METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
Study 1: Experiments in public settings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
Study 2: Interviews in residential settings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
FINDINGS OF STUDY 1 (EXPERIMENTS IN PUBLIC SETTINGS) . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Reactions to fly-by vs. hovering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Making sense of a drone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Participants’ perception of altitude and distance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
The perception of safety distance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
FINDINGS OF STUDY 2 (RESIDENTIAL SETTING) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Informational privacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Spatial privacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
13
13
14
15
17
18
18
21
Uncertainty . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
Views on recreational use of micro drones . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
CONCLUSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
–3–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0004.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
INTRODUCTION
The public will play a vital role in shaping the future of the drone sector. The sector’s
fate is tied to factors such as the capacity to serve the public and convince it that dro-
nes can benefit society, the ability to ensure that drones are used in a safe and con-
siderate manner, and the readiness and effectiveness of the sector to address public
concerns, such as safety and privacy. This report addresses public reactions to drones
in residential and public settings and the concerns they raised. We present the results
of two studies conducted as part of a collaborative project between the University of
Southern Denmark (SDU), Aalborg University (AAU) and the Danish Transport, Con-
struction and Housing Authority. The report builds upon and supplements the research
conducted in the initial phase of the project (Bajde et al. 2017).
The research questions were formulated as follows:
• How do people respond to the presence of drones in public areas? How are these re-
sponses impacted by the altitude at which the drone is flying and other factors, such
as the size of the drone and the nature of its use?
• How do people respond to the presence of drones in the vicinity of their homes? How
are these responses impacted by the altitude at which the drone is flying and other
factors, such as the size of the drone and the nature of its use?
• Does the presence of drones provoke privacy concerns? If so, what kind of privacy
concerns and under what conditions?
We pose these questions to provide insights for policy makers and businesses who are
interested in designing regulations and drone solutions that are attentive to public
concerns. The report provides an overview of the work process and the main results of
two studies, conducted by two independent research teams. The first study looks at
reactions to drones in public areas, while the second study investigates reactions to
drones in people’s private home settings.
–4–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0005.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
SUMMARY OF MAIN RESULTS
Our study of reactions to the presence of drones in public settings shows a dependency
on the perceived purpose and legitimacy of the drone, which is mediated by the percei-
ved distance and duration of the drone’s presence. A drone that flies by sufficiently far
away and has a clear and legitimate purpose does not cause people to interrupt their
activities, and is not seen as problematic. However, a drone that lingers in public areas,
comes too close to bystanders is disruptive or intrusive if its purpose and legitimacy
is unclear. People might infer that they are being filmed or followed, or feel unsafe or
bothered. When seeing a drone, bystanders therefore try to ascertain its purpose and
legitimacy by either trying to interpret its flight pattern, lights, or design; or by looking
for a pilot to hold accountable.
Figure 1:
Determinants of reactions to drones in public settings
PERCEIVED
HEIGHT & DISTANCE
UNCLEAR PURPOSE
UNCERTAIN LEGITIMACY
DISTURBANCE & UNCERTAINTY
Crucially, by observing a drone from the ground, it is hard for members of the public to
discern how high a drone is flying, what it is doing, and who is responsible for it. Uncer-
tainty and disturbance occur if purpose and legitimacy of drones are not easily identi-
fiable. Current drone designs offer little indication of the responsible entity (e.g. public
authorities, commercial or private users), the mission or direction of the flight (e.g.
bystanders cannot tell the meaning of red and blue lights on the drone), and whether
or not current regulations are being followed (e.g. even if bystanders know the permit-
ted flight level, they cannot estimate the altitude that a drone is flying at).
We conclude in particular that low-flying drones often interrupt social activities, and
that current designs and regulations make it difficult to assess purpose and legitimacy,
causing uncertainty and disturbance.
–5–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0006.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
Our study of reactions to drones in residential settings shows that public concerns
regarding privacy cannot be reduced to the problem of unauthorised capture of per-
sonal information or images (i.e., violation of
informational privacy).
Instead, privacy
concerns are better seen through the prism of the
privacy triangle
comprising three
dimensions: informational privacy, spatial privacy and uncertainty. Our data stresses
the importance of spatial privacy, which refers to the protection of one’s private space
from uninvited intrusions and threats. In the case of drones, violations of spatial priva-
cy relate to feeling threatened, or intruded upon by the noise and physical presence of
the drone in private space. Problems of spatial privacy become particularly acute when
large drones overfly private gardens or houses at lower altitudes (e.g., 25 metres).
Our study shows that the noise and visual design of the drone play an important role
in shaping the public’s experiences of intrusion, thus providing insight into how drone
designers and regulators can mitigate problems related to spatial privacy.
Figure 2:
Privacy triangle
NOT KNOWING
(uncertainty)
INTRUDED
UPON
(Spatial privacy)
OBSERVED
(Informational privacy)
Informational privacy and spatial privacy are significantly impacted by the third ele-
ment of the privacy tringle, uncertainty – the public’s inability to understand what is
going on in the vicinity of their home. We find that the inability to obtain information,
such as information about who is operating a drone and for what purpose it is being
flown over their house, significantly diminishes people’s sense of privacy in both public
and private residential settings. This problem can be mitigated by creating identifica-
tion systems for drones that could be used by citizens to access real-time information
about specific cases of drone use, as well as by improved communication with the pub-
lic about the costs and benefits of drone use.
–6–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0007.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
Finally, we find that the size and purpose of the drone plays an important role in sha-
ping public reactions. The problems of spatial privacy and uncertainty are not as acute
when it comes to recreational use of smaller drones. Participants in the study felt that
people should have the right to play with drones in their own gardens and on their own
property, as long as they do not film, photograph or otherwise observe or monitor ne-
ighbours. Reactions to recreational use of small drones in public space, such as public
parks and playgrounds, were less univocal, indicating the need for further research and
careful consideration of how this aspect of drone use is regulated and communicated
to the public.
–7–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0008.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
RESEARCH DESIGN & METHODOLOGY
The research project was comprised of two studies, conducted by two independent
teams. One team (Bajde, Bruun, Sommer, Nøjgaard, Christensen and Jensen) explo-
red public concerns with drone use in residential areas through interviews with parti-
cipants exposed to drones in the context of their homes and gardens. Another team
(Woermann, Gahrn-Andersen, Kirschner and Bucher) investigated people’s reactions to
drone use in public areas via field experiments in public settings through video analysis
and post-experiment interviews.
STUDY 1: EXPERIMENTS IN PUBLIC SETTINGS
The study design was based on a review of the state-of-the-art of human-drone and
human-robot interaction, as well as video analysis of footage capturing naturally
occurring encounters with drones available on social media platforms. The results led
to a reflexive experimental design: participants were given tasks such as solving an
IQ test or playing a game, and then drones were flown unannounced into their vicini-
ty. This forced participants to make sense of the drone and react in ways they deem
appropriate. Building on the tradition of ethnomethodological experiments (Garfinkel
1967), studies in human-machine interaction (Suchman 1987), and workplace studi-
es (Hindmarsh et al. 2000), sensemaking and reactions were captured on video from
several angles and analysed in conjunction with post-experiment focus group intervie-
ws. This allows participants’ stated attitude towards drones to be contrasted with their
in-situ behaviour.
The experiments were conducted in group and solo settings with either a commerci-
al drone (category 2; DJI Matrice 600) or a toy drone (below 250 g; Silverlit Spy drone
II, and Harbok Nano). To understand reactions to different altitudes, the category 1B
drone conducted an unannounced fly-by at 75, 50, and 20 metres, before hovering over
the participants.
In the case of the
75 m
toy drone, the pilot
was visible in addi-
50 m
tion to the drone,
and walked nearby
20 m
the participants.
This comparative
design allowed the
effect of the pilot
being in or out of
plain sight to be
established.
–8–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0009.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
All experiments were conducted at the University of Southern Denmark in a recreatio-
nal setting. After the experiments, 16 focus group interviews with the participants were
conducted (72 participants in total). The experiments lasted between 12 and 20 mi-
nutes and produced over 9 hours of video footage in total. Focus groups lasted between
25 and 30 minutes and were transcribed verbatim (7 hours of recordings and approx.
210 pages of transcript in total). The videos and transcripts were triangulated as refle-
xive depending data (Flick 2009).
Pilot
Drone
–9–
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0010.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
In total, 72 individuals participated in the study. Table 1 shows the sample according
to average age and gender. Participants were recruited through bulletin boards, soci-
al media and word-of-mouth among students at the University of Southern Denmark
from all faculties and levels who did not have expert knowledge of drones. Participants
received financial compensation.
Pre-screening guaranteed a diverse sample and ensured that all safety regulations
could be accounted for. As is customary for reflexive experimental designs, the sample
is not intended to be representative with regard to individual socio-demographic cha-
racteristics, but instead allows for ecological validity (Cicourel 1982) by focusing on a
social group (local university students) while ensuring diversity within the sample of
interactional units (Denzin 1969), for example by including different nationalities, gen-
ders, and social backgrounds.
Table 1:
Description of participants in Study 1
Participants (Total)
Average Age
72
23.56
Number of
participants
Gender
Female
Male
Danish
Not Danish
34
38
32
40
Share of
participants (%)
47.2
52.8
44.4
55.6
Nationality
– 10 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0011.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
STUDY 2: INTERVIEWS IN RESIDENTIAL SETTINGS
The residential setting interviews were conducted in two stages. In the first stage par-
ticipants were interviewed in their homes to obtain a base understanding of their level
of knowledge about drones, their attitudes toward drones and their personal views
on privacy. Some days after the initial interview the participants were visited again to
conduct a drone test in their private gardens (all participants lived in attached or deta-
ched houses with gardens) and a second interview aimed at obtaining their reactions
to the drones and reflections on the presence of drones in the vicinity of their homes.
– 11 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0012.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
75 m
50 m
25 m
The drone overflying par-
ticipants’ gardens was a
Matrice 600 at the altitu-
des of 25m, 50m and 75m.
The participants were
also shown a smaller toy
drone (Silverlit Spy drone
II; below 250g), and asked
to share their views on
recreational drone use in
the vicinity of their homes
and other places, such as
public areas and parks.
In total, 16 participants who were not experts in drones, were recruited through per-
sonal networks (see Table 1). Although not representative of the Danish population,
the sample includes significant diversity across gender (50% of the participants are
female/male), age (24-83, average 41), residential areas (6 locations spread across
larger-city central and suburban areas, as well as village settings), and occupation.
Table 2:
Description of participants in study
2
Participant
Jane
Michael
Tina
Jesper
Eva
Anton
Erik
Emma
Peter
Mette
Minh
Hans
Dennis
Tine
Ellen
Freja
Gender
F
M
F
M
F
M
M
F
M
F
M
M
M
F
F
F
Age
30
24
28
29
59
55
42
52
30
26
37
83
47
27
58
28
Type of residence
Attached house with shared garden in urban centre
Attached house with shared garden in urban centre
Attached house with private garden in urban centre
Attached house with private garden in urban centre
Attached house with private garden in urban centre
Attached house with private garden in urban centre
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Detached house with private garden in a village
Detached house with private garden in a village
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Detached house with private garden in a village
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Detached house with private garden in suburban area
Attached house with private garden in suburban area
– 12 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
All interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed in full. The names and addresses
were anonymised to protect the privacy of the participants. The transcriptions were
analysed by reducing the data into shorter summaries, based on which patterns were
identified and tested across the data set.
FINDINGS OF STUDY 1 (EXPERIMENTS IN PUBLIC SETTINGS)
The field experiments produced great homogeneity in results, despite variation in the
types of drones used, types of public settings, and participants conducting either
social activities (such as playing different games or sports) or focused activities (such
as answering an IQ test) either alone or in groups. Further, both pre-reflexive reactions
(such as briefly glancing at a drone while continuing the current activity) that were
captured through video analysis, and participants’ explicit reflections in post-experi-
ment interviews confirm the same general pattern.
REACTIONS TO FLY-BY VS. HOVERING
Reactions to the unannounced presence of drones can be divided into two categories:
First, as drones fly by at a certain distance, participants react to the noise and esta-
blish the distant presence of the drone by briefly glancing up. Participants engaged
in an activity capturing their attention will not react outwardly other than this short
moment of visually attending to the drone. After the drone has been established as
a background feature of the situation in that participants consider it too far away to
require further attention, participants no longer react outwardly to the drone even if it
flies by a second time. Reflecting upon their behaviour, participants explain that they
thought the drone had “nothing to do with me” or their current activity, and given its
distance and short presence, was not considered intrusive.
Second, the reaction pattern changes markedly once a drone flies at a distance that
participants consider “close to me” (see below for further discussion) – now the presen-
ce of the drone requires attention and invokes a range of reactions. Participants start
looking at the drone for longer periods and visibly alter their bodily posture so that their
observing the drone becomes apparent for others in their group. This, in turn, will cause
other group members to likewise look at the drone. Participants frequently express a
negative stance towards the drone by showing negative facial expressions, uttering ne-
gative emotions, and expressing bewilderment, curiosity, or disturbance. If interacting
in a group, participants start to communicate about the drone, sharing their negative
experience and eventually engaging in sensemaking regarding the possible purpose
and legitimacy of the drone.
– 13 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
MAKING SENSE OF A DRONE
A drone that lingers in public areas and comes too close to bystanders is disruptive or
intrusive because it requires attention and sensemaking. Participants speculate that
they are being filmed or followed, or feel unsafe or bothered.
When seeing a drone, bystanders try to ascertain its purpose and legitimacy by either
trying to interpret its flight pattern, lights, or design. Furthermore, most participants
begin looking around in order to identify the pilot of the drone in order to hold them
accountable. In several tests, we intentionally hid the pilot from view. This caused par-
ticipants to either consider other members of the research team as the pilots, or cau-
sed repeated visual searching for a pilot in the vicinity. In no case did the participants
seem content with what they could understand from just looking at the drone itself. In
the post-experiment interviews, participants univocally explained that it is decisive for
them to know who is flying the drone, or who is responsible for it. For example, parti-
cipants would trust a fireman to fly the drone safely, and would accept drones prac-
tically regardless of where and how they fly if they knew that they were being used to
help saving lives. In contrast, if they consider a drone to be piloted by an untrustworthy
person, the size and flight pattern of the drone does not make much of a difference:
they are univocally rejected.
Crucially, by observing a drone from the ground, it is hard for participants to under-
stand how high a drone is flying, what it is doing, and who is responsible for it. The
experiments unexpectedly confronted participants with drones and offered little indi-
cation of the responsible entity (e.g. public authorities, commercial or private users).
Only after participants concluded that the drones where probably a part of the experi-
ments, did they pay less attention and become more comfortable with the drone. In a
naturally occurring situation where members of the public would be entirely unable to
ascertain the purpose and legitimacy of a drone, more pronounced feelings of insecuri-
ty and stronger reactions seem probable.
The drone designs used offered participants little opportunity to understand the purpo-
se or direction of flight of the drone. For example, participants could not tell the mea-
ning of red and blue lights on the drone, or were surprised by the hovering and sudden
changes of direction. Additionally, participants could not estimate whether or not
current regulations were being followed by the drone and its pilot. However, in line with
sociological research, participants expressed that checking if a pilot follows the rules
and laws is an important strategy to assess the trustworthiness of the drone, and the
safety of the situation more generally (Goffman 1963). For example, if even relatively
minor formal or informal rules of normal behaviour in public are being visibly ignored
by someone, bystanders will keep their distance and monitor that person. However,
with regard to a drone, people cannot asses if the drone is behaving like a ‘good citizen’
– 14 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
or not, because they neither know the rules nor are able to tell if the rules are being
followed. This is particularly true with regard to rules about minimum flight altitude.
PARTICIPANTS’ PERCEPTION OF ALTITUDE AND DISTANCE
Participants were generally unable to estimate the drone’s flying height. Indeed, they
failed to notice that the drone was flown at three different altitudes during each expe-
riment (i.e. 75 metres, 50 metres and 20 metres). While some participants experienced
the changing altitudes during the various fly-bys, others did not notice any change.
However, all participants noticed when the drone began to descend directly above
them:
“I felt it like above me a lot of the time, I know it was like flying around but I
think it like stopped, right?” (Vera, 24)
Further, participants generally reported on the drone’s altitude in relative terms, by
using phrases such as “closer to” and “further away from”:
“I was not really paying attention to the drone; only when its sound increa-
sed if it got closer for some reason. Then I looked up. But I haven’t been pay-
ing attention to where it was flying or anything…” (Birgit, 23)
“Especially the noise [was distracting]. Because even though [the drone] was
far away, you could still hear it like it was close, or near.” (Line, 20)
As these quotes reflect, there was a general tendency to infer the drone’s altitude and
distance based on the noise level. In the follow-up interviews, participants were asked
to use the metric system to estimate the drone’s varying altitudes. Given the absence
of measurement tools and altitude/distance markings, however, their estimations were
incorrect. They generally had the impression that the low-flying drone was much lower,
providing estimates as little as five metres when in fact the drone was flying at 25 me-
tres:
“I didn’t pay attention, but she (i.e. another participant) said like 20 metres
[…] so 15 [meters up] maybe? I think it should be higher because you never
know if the person faint or something and the drone is like… yeah, I guess
higher…“ (Vera, 24)
Thus, participants were unable to estimate the drone’s altitude. Nevertheless, they
generally emphasised that drones should not fly “too close” to them if they are to feel
safe in their presence. We conclude that perceptions of safety and intrusiveness of
drones are not a linear function of the absolute flight level or distance, but instead de-
– 15 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0016.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
pend on a) the noise level and b) the perceived appropriateness of the drone given the
current situation. In other words, whether or not a drone is considered as “flying too
close to me” depends on the setting (e.g. public vs. private space, recreational or work
environment, etc.) and whether the drone fits this setting. The closer the drone flies to
them, the more participants feel the need to trust the drone pilot not to put them in
danger or do something reckless. Therefore, participants become more active in try-
ing to ascertain the legitimacy of the flight and the trustworthiness of the pilot as the
drone comes closer.
“For me it was the same. When it was very close, it was very distracting. But
when it was further away, then it was not that bad.” (Ronny, 23)
“When they [i.e. drones] come near me, I have this thing that they will bump
into me so I have to, you know, just kind of take a look. So, I was kind of
distracted, because the thing is when it was far away, it took me a while to
understand where the noise came from, like what it was about.” (Liza, 23)
– 16 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0017.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
THE PERCEPTION OF SAFETY DISTANCE
Generally, there was little consent amongst participants regarding the optimal safe-
ty distance for a bystander from a drone. Thus, their opinions varied as the following
passage shows:
Lisa, 29: “[The small drone] won’t bother me if it is far away from me. Or,
not far-far, but if is it in the area I am in.”
Interviewer: “So how far is far? Like how would you say?”
Linda, 22: “Eh… maybe… three meters, four metres, away from me.”
Fernando, 25: “Well, I would say more. Like, eh… yeah 20 metres.”
Interviewer: “20 meters, okay. So, was this drone closer than that?”
Fernando: “Yeah I think he was like, was it 10 metres from me in, at the end.”
Andreas, 25: “Well, I would say the guy playing with the drone I would say if
he was closer than 10 metres I would think it was weird but the drone I would
want like 50 metres away.”
To conclude, our findings reveal that people are concerned with the distance and al-
titude of drones whenever they either feel annoyed by them or that their private space
is being violated. In most cases, the noise made by the drone was the first thing that
triggered these negative feelings. Further, it is impossible to specify an optimal altitude
and distance since different people have different thresholds and conceptions. Howe-
ver, it is evident that the context of the encounter is of decisive importance. As some of
the participants noted, their reactions would have been different had they encountered
the drones in everyday (i.e. non-experimental) settings without knowing either the
drone’s purpose or who was flying it.
– 17 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
FINDINGS OF STUDY 2 (INTERVIEWS IN RESIDENTIAL SETTINGS)
The 16 interviewees have varied conceptions of home and privacy. Nevertheless, some
patterns can be discerned in the expectation that participants have regarding privacy
in the context of their homes. The home is regularly seen as a private space that inclu-
des both the house and the adjoining garden, a space where 1) people can relax and
enjoy life free from disturbances and intrusions of uninvited others, 2) people are freed
from the gaze and judgements of others, and 3) people have control over things and
make their own decisions.
These conceptions and experiences of home and private space shape the expectations
and concerns that people voice about the use of drones in the vicinity of their home. The
newly acquired data permits us to extend the findings of our initial study (Bajde et al,
2017). We identify three dimensions of privacy concerns – the privacy triangle (Figure 1):
1) informational privacy
2) spatial privacy
3) uncertainty
Explanation and evidence for each dimension is presented below. We pay particular
attention to the dimension of spatial privacy as this dimension was less apparent prior
to the study and was found to be particularly central to reactions to the presence of a
Matrice 600 drone. In the last part of this section we discuss the participants’ reactions
to recreational use of smaller drones and their views on recreational drone regulation.
The participants had very limited experience with larger commercial drones, so a sub-
stantial majority was surprised by the size of a Matrice 600. Their reactions to experi-
encing the drone in action were mixed as they simultaneously found the drone impres-
sive and troublesome. While far less common when the drone stayed at the height of
75 metres, negative reactions became dominant at the lower altitudes, 25 metres in
particular. This conclusion holds for all three elements of the privacy triangle. We begin
by considering informational privacy.
INFORMATIONAL PRIVACY
Informational privacy refers to concerns that the drone will capture unauthorized
personal information (Finn et al. 2014). The most common concern expressed is being
photographed or filmed in one’s own house or garden.
“I think I would have a hard time if they [drones] fly in front of the windows.
I have my boundaries… I also think that people should have privacy in their
house and likewise in their own gardens, so that you can wear a bikini
without being photographed from above.” (Tine, 27)
– 18 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0019.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
In Tine’s case the concern is illustrated with the example of unauthorized capture of
sensitive personal information. Other participants list concerns such as being spied
upon by governmental institutions or nosy neighbours. The distance at which drone
surveillance and information capture take place, can play an important role.
“Oh boy, now you can actually see it… There are almost too many details…
You think that when it’s way up there [75m], then it only gives you [only] a
rough overview. I know that’s a bit naïve, because it [the drone’s camera] is
capable of so many things, but… you’re thinking that it’s not that close.”
(Ellen, 58)
Ellen feels that at 25m the drone becomes very visible, and at the same time she be-
comes very visible to the drone. She notes that while it might be naïve to underesti-
mate the drone’s capacity to observe from a greater height, she still feels much more
uncomfortable with the prospect of being observed from the lower altitude (25m).
There is a difference between actually being observed and having the feeling of being
observed. Irrespective of whether the drone is actually collecting information about
her, Ellen feels that she is observed. As discussed in the section devoted to uncertain-
ty, not knowing much about the drone’s capacities and purpose makes the problem of
informational privacy worse.
– 19 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0020.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
– 20 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
In addition to the distance, Ellen also points out that the movement of the drone is an
important factor in determining the negative experience of being observed. It is when
the drone hovers over her house that she feels most observed. Similar concerns regar-
ding informational privacy are expressed by other participants, who fear being obser-
ved and recorded while inhabiting a space that should not be subject to the eyes and
cameras of unwelcomed strangers (drones included).
SPATIAL PRIVACY
Spatial privacy refers to being protected from uninvited intrusions into one’s physical
and/or psychological sphere (Laurie 1999), in a manner that disrupts a person’s right to
be left alone (Solove 2008). In the case of drones, spatial privacy might relate to dis-
comforting feelings of being disturbed by the physical presence and noise of a drone
in one’s private space, or even feeling physically threatened by drones (e.g. fears of
injury).
We find that the problems of spatial privacy are significantly reduced when the drone
overflies a private garden or house at 75 metres. In contrast, they become particularly
acute at the lowest altitude. While some participants begin to express annoyance with
the drone’s presence at the 50 metre altitude, virtually all participants express some
concerns when the drone descends to 25 metres:
“Now I think it’s getting way too close, and it’s making more noise. So, now
it’s starting to become disturbing… It’s entering into my personal bubble.”
(Freja, 28)
I’m sick of the noise right now. Definitely. Now it’s turning into a somewhat
more annoying noise than when it was higher up [50m and 75m]. (Erik, 42)
“When it comes a bit too close… You are not able to control the situation. It
is rather uncomfortable. It’s a bit monster-like… It’s a bit spider-like.” (Hans,
83)
The participants feel discomforted by the presence of the drone at the 25 metre altitu-
de. They point out that the drone intrudes into their private space with a disturbing
noise, and with its visual presence. As indicated by Freja’s reference to the drone’s inva-
sion into her “personal bubble”, the drone does not invade private territory in the usual
sense of passing across the ground boundary demarcating private land. Instead private
territory is experienced in 3-dimensional terms: as a space that can be equally distur-
bed by entities that fly (too low).
– 21 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
There is no universal rule of thumb as to how high the bubble of private space extends
(i.e., the participants do not have a clear sense how high their private space reaches.
Further, the size of the bubble depends on the properties and behavior of the drone.
Our data shows that in the case of a large, noisy drone such as a Matrice 600, the pri-
vate bubble seems to reach at least 25 metres for most of the participants. In contrast,
spatial privacy works differently with smaller, less noisy drones, where the participants
are more willing to tolerate the drone at a lower altitude, as long as it is not filming
them or compromising their safety.
In summary, the participants experience the space enveloping their home (house and
adjoining garden) as a private space, where they are to be left alone and undisturbed.
Their claims are not based on explicit appeals to property rights (i.e., owning the space
around the house), but rather to culturally shared norms and psychological experiences
of “spaces of non-intrusion” (Hoeyer 2010, p. 271). Technologies such as drones, reopen
questions of “domestic boundaries” (Garvey 2005). Prior to the presence of the drone,
the participants had rarely considered what separates their private space from the col-
lectively owned airspace. The tests we conducted, compelled the participants to figure
out (in practice) at what point a drone crosses the borders of their private space. Put
differently, they figure out in situ at what point the “U-space” (European Commission
2017) occupied by drones starts to impinge upon private “Me-space”.
For example, there are marked differences in reactions between the higher altitudes
and the lowest altitude. While at 75 and 50 metres the sound of the drone is likened to
the noise of street traffic or lawnmowers, at the 25 metre altitude the participants be-
come bothered and annoyed by the noise of the drone. When expressing their discom-
fort with the proximity of the drone, participants describe the drone as “monstrous”
and “otherworldly”, that is, as something that does not belong in the context of their
home. While we can assume that such reactions might dissipate as drones become
more common, our results clearly indicate that both the sound and the visual design
of the drone plays an important role in shaping the public’s response to drones (e.g.,
feelings of vulnerability, intrusion and annoyance).
Finally, although the tests were organized in a manner that removes any doubt regar-
ding who is using the drone and for what purpose (i.e., the participants knew that the
drone is being operated by the research team), the participants regularly raised the
issue of uncertainty in regard to the purpose of drones, which we discuss next.
UNCERTAINTY
Not knowing who is operating a drone, and for what purpose, significantly exacerba-
tes feelings of privacy violation. As argued by Jesper (29), this relates to “wanting to
know where it [the drone] came from… Who’s at the other end. Because that’s what
– 22 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
maybe affects me the most, when it comes to privacy.” In a similar manner, several
participants argue that not being able to tell if the drone has a camera, and whether
it is recording, and who is observing/recording, causes considerable distress even if no
leakage of private information is actually taking place. As explained by Jane (26), bet-
ter access to information can help mitigate this problem: “[I]f you just tell people what
you are using it [the drone] for, police work or something like that, then I don’t think it
would bother anyone. I think it [privacy concerns] very much depends on the informa-
tion about why they [the drones] are there.” Put differently, privacy concerns can be
mitigated by providing information about the purpose of the drone.
“Not knowing”, on the other hand, exacerbates the experience of violation of both
informational and spatial privacy. Being informed about the presence of a drone in
advance, or at least being able to obtain information about it once it is noticed, might
enable participants to cope with the presence of drones in the vicinity of their home.
For example, Michael (26) argues that having information would enable him to mi-
nimize the disturbance: “Then you can prepare yourself that you shouldn’t run about
buck-naked on your patio.”
Others like Mette (26) argue that in the absence of specific information they would not
know how to react or what to do in case of drone intrusion. Therefore, providing ad-
vance notice that drones will be taking photographs or filming in the vicinity of one’s
home, and helping people to identify the drone and its mission, can help people reas-
sert a sense of control over their private space and mitigate the disturbing effects of
drone presence. In addition to amplifying informational and spatial privacy concerns,
uncertainty might be experienced as a fundamental aspect of privacy in its own right.
Additional analysis is needed to test this hypothesis.
Finally, our data also hints that public reactions and willingness to tolerate a decrease
in privacy due to drone presence, are mediated by the overall knowledge about and
disposition toward drones. Problems of uncertainty are exacerbated by the public’s
limited overall knowledge about drones, and uncertainty about the actual benefits and
hazards they pose. When people perceive drones to be beneficial to them or to society
at large, the willingness to tolerate and compromise increases. This stresses the need
for public institutions and industry to communicate with the public and engage in
emergent discussions about the benefits and costs of drones to society and individual
citizens.
VIEWS ON RECREATIONAL USE OF MICRO DRONES
After witnessing the flight of a Matrice600, the participants were shown a smaller
toy drone and asked to share their views on recreational use of smaller drones by ne-
– 23 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0024.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
ighbourhood residents. Participants express a high degree of tolerance for use of toy
drones in residential settings under certain conditions.
“I don’t think the toy drone is a problem. I mean, children should be allowed
to play. So, if the neighborhood kids play with drones and one ends up in our
garden – oh well. It doesn’t matter! It’s like if a football ends up in there [in
your garden]. Yes, they are welcome to do that.” (Hans 83)
There is a general strong consent among the participants that people should be allow-
ed to play with drones in their own gardens. The participants feel less bothered by a
drone, if it is small and less noisy. However, their tolerance has limits. After expressing
his approval of toy drone use, Hans stresses that this does not apply to drones flying
around the neighbourhood with a camera. His opinion corresponds to a clear pattern
across participant reactions to toy drones, that drones with cameras should be restric-
ted to the users’ own gardens, and unauthorized filming of neighbours was entirely
unacceptable. Several participants find it disturbing that the purpose of using a toy
drone equipped with camera is unclear (uncertainty), and various “transgressive” mi-
suses can be imagined.
Nevertheless, there is a reluctance to call for strict regulation, and communication
between neighbours and reliance on established neighbourhood norms that apply to
other play activities (e.g., kite-flying) is seen as a better solution to addressing the
potential problems arising from recreational drone use. The use of larger, more dange-
rous drones is seen as inappropriate outside of designated areas that ensure safety and
minimize the disturbing effects of drone noise.
Reactions to toy drone use in public spaces such as playgrounds or parks are mixed.
Although the participants express a higher degree of tolerance for smaller toy drones
(compared to larger commercial drones) and tend to be less concerned with being
observed or filmed in public space than in private space, some participants voice
concerns regarding safety, noise and the appropriateness of technological objects in
public spaces designed for children to play safely, or for adults to relax in natural sur-
roundings. As a result, participants voicing higher levels of concern suggest that drones
should only be used in designated public areas.
– 24 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
CONCLUSION
The first study has two clear implications for effective regulation. Firstly, drone flights
at 50 metres and above have little effect, while flights at 20 metres disturb social
activities. Secondly, regulators and drone designers need to think about ways to make
it easy for people to understand what a drone is doing, if it is legitimate, and who is
responsible. Uncertainty and disturbance occur primarily in situations in which the
purpose and legitimacy of a drone are not easily identifiable. If regulators and desig-
ners can succeed in making drones easier to understand and to trust, they would be
better accepted by the public. The experimental methods used in this study provide an
effective toolbox to test a range of possible future solutions to these challenges.
The second study identifies a similar problem of uncertainty in a residential setting. By
delving deeper into the public’s privacy concerns, we uncover two main concerns: infor-
mational privacy and spatial privacy. We show that both forms of privacy are impacted
by the public’s uncertainty regarding drones and their use. What is more, we discover
that experiences of privacy concerns vary according to the height at which a drone
flies and the type of drone used (e.g., large commercial drone vs. smaller toy drone).
Our data indicates that the presence of larger drones at a 25-metre altitude signifi-
cantly impinges on people’s private space (understood in 3-dimenisonal terms). While
this problem is less significant in the case of smaller toy drones, informational privacy
concerns do not diminish, if toy drones are perceived to record photographs and video
footage.
In-depth interviews conducted prior and after direct exposure to drones were found to
be an effective tool in investigating how the presence of drones impacts the public’s
understanding of private space and concerns about privacy. Combined with observati-
onal methods, this approach offers valuable insight into public reactions to drone use
in urban areas.
– 25 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
REFERENCES
Bajde, D., Bruun, M. H., Sommer, J. K., Waltorp, K. (2017).
”General public’s privacy
concerns regarding drone use in residential and public areas.”
University of Southern
Denmark.
Cicourel, A. V. (1982).
“Interviews, Surveys, and the Problem of Ecological Validity.”
The American Sociologist 17 (1):11–20.
Denzin, N. K. (1969).
“Symbolic Interactionism and Ethnomethodology: A Proposed
Synthesis.”
American Sociological Review 34 (6):922–34.
European Commission (2017).
Aviation: Commission is taking the European drone sec-
tor to new heights.
Press release.
Finn, R.L, Wright, D., Donovan, A., Jacques, L and De Hert, P. (2014).
Study on priva-
cy, data protection and ethical risks in civil Remotely Piloted Aircraft Systems operati-
ons.
D3.3: Final report for the European Commission.
Garfinkel, H. (1967).
Studies in Ethnomethodology.
Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Garvey, P. (2005).
Domestic boundaries: Privacy, visibility and the Norwegian window.
Journal of Material Culture, 10(2), 157-176.
Goffman, E. (1963).
Behavior in Public Places.
Glencoe: Free Press.
Hindmarsh, J., C. Heath, and P. Luff (2000).
Workplace Studies: Recovering Work Pra-
ctice and Informing System Design.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hoeyer, K. (2010).
The role of privacy and informed consent in Danish and Swedish
biobank practices: Exploring donor perspectives.
Medical Law International, 2010, 10,
269–285.
Laurie, G. T. (1999).
In defence of ignorance: Genetic information and the right not to
know.
European Journal of Health Law 6(2), 119-132.
Solove, D. J. (2008).
Understanding privacy. Cambridge.
MA: Harvard University Press
Suchman, Lucy (1987).
Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-Machine
Communication.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Graphical resources provided by Freepik.
– 26 –
TRU, Alm.del - 2017-18 - Bilag 136: Resultaterne af undersøgelser vedrørende befolkningens viden om og holdning til brugen af droner i offentlige områder og beboelsesområder, fra transport-, bygnings- og boligministeren
1848319_0027.png
PUBLIC REACTIONS TO DRONE USE IN RESIDENTIAL AND PUBLIC AREAS
– 27 –
UNIVERSITY OF
SOUTHERN DENMARK